CHAPTER 13: EXPERIMENTS & OBSERVATIONAL STUDIES A P S TAT I S T I C S
36 Slides238.78 KB
CHAPTER 13: EXPERIMENTS & OBSERVATIONAL STUDIES A P S TAT I S T I C S
OBSERVATIONAL STUDIES In an observational study, researchers don’t assign choices; they simply observe them. The text’s example looked at the relationship between music education and grades. Since the researchers did not assign students to get music education and simply observed students by recording the choices they made the outcomes-it was an observational study.
OBSERVATIONAL STUDIES (CONT.) Because researchers in the text example first identified subjects who studied music and then collected data on their past grades, this was a retrospective study. Had the researchers identified subjects in advance and collected data as events unfolded, the study would have been a prospective study.
OBSERVATIONAL STUDIES (CONT.) Observational studies are valuable for discovering trends and possible relationships. However, it is not possible for observational studies, whether prospective or retrospective, to demonstrate a causal relationship.
RANDOMIZED, COMPARATIVE EXPERIMENTS An experiment is a study design that allows us to prove a cause-and-effect relationship. FINALLY In an experiment, the experimenter must identify at least one explanatory variable, called a factor, to manipulate and at least one response variable to measure. An experiment: Manipulates factor levels to create treatments. Randomly assigns subjects to these treatment levels. Compares the responses of the subject groups across treatment levels.
RANDOMIZED, COMPARATIVE EXPERIMENTS (CONT.) To sum it up In an experiment, the experimenter actively and deliberately manipulates the factors to control the details of the possible treatments, and assigns the subjects to those treatments at random. The experimenter then observes the response variable and compares responses for different groups of subjects who have been treated differently.
RANDOMIZED, COMPARATIVE EXPERIMENTS (CONT.) In general, the individuals on whom or which we experiment are called experimental units. When humans are involved, they are commonly called subjects or participants. Everything else (rats, days, petri dishes of bacteria, etc.) are called experimental units. The specific values that the experimenter chooses for a factor are called the levels of the factor. Example: The researcher chooses to observe her participants sleeping 4, 6, and 8 hours. A treatment is a combination of specific levels from all the factors that an experimental unit receives.
THE FOUR PRINCIPLES OF EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN 1. Control: We control sources of variation other than the factors we are testing by making conditions as similar as possible for all treatment groups. 2. Randomize: Randomization allows us to equalize the effects of unknown or uncontrollable sources of variation. It does not eliminate the effects of these sources, but it spreads them out across the treatment levels so that we can see past them. Without randomization, you do not have a valid experiment and will not be able to use the powerful methods of Statistics to draw conclusions from your study.
THE FOUR PRINCIPLES OF EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN (CONT.) 3. Replicate: Repeat the experiment, applying the treatments to a number of subjects. The outcome of an experiment on a single subject is an anecdote/sketch, not data. When the experimental group is not a representative sample of the population of interest, we might want to replicate an entire experiment for different groups, in different situations, etc. Replication of an entire experiment with the controlled sources of variation at different levels is an essential step in science.
THE FOUR PRINCIPLES OF EXPERIMENTAL DESIGN (CONT.) 4. Block: Sometimes, attributes of the experimental units that we are not studying and that we can’t control may nevertheless affect the outcomes of an experiment. If we group similar individuals together and then randomize within each of these blocks, we can remove much of the variability due to the difference among the blocks. Note: Blocking is an important compromise between randomization and control, but, unlike the first three principles, is not required in an experimental design.
DIAGRAMS OF EXPERIMENTS It’s often helpful to diagram the procedure of an experiment. The following diagram emphasizes the random allocation of subjects to treatment groups, the separate treatments applied to these groups, and the ultimate comparison of results:
DOES THE DIFFERENCE MAKE A DIFFERENCE? How large do the differences need to be to say that there is a difference in the treatments? Differences that are larger than we’d get just from the randomization alone are called statistically significant. We’ll talk more about statistical significance later. For now, the important point is that a difference is statistically significant if we don’t believe that it’s likely to have occurred only by chance.
EXAMPLE At one time, “gastric freezing” was used to treat people with peptic ulcers. An inflatable bladder was inserted down the esophagus and into the stomach, and then a cold liquid was pumped into the bladder. Now you can find the following notice on the Internet site of a major insurance company: [Our company] does not cover gastric freezing (intragastric hypothermia) for chronic peptic ulcer disease Gastric freezing for chronic peptic ulcer disease is a nonsurgical treatment which was popular about 20 years ago but now is seldom performed. It has been abandoned due to a high complication rate, only temporary improvement experienced by patients, and a lack of effectiveness when tested by double-blind, controlled clinical trials.
EXAMPLE (CONT.) What did the “controlled clinical trial” (experiment) probably look like? (We will get to double-blind soon, so disregard it at this time). 1. What was the factor in this experiment? 2. What was the response variable? 3. What were the treatments? 4. How did researchers decide which subjects received which treatment? 5. Were the results statistically significant?
CONTROL TREATMENTS Often, we want to compare a situation involving a specific treatment to the status quo situation. A baseline (“business as usual”) measurement is called a control treatment, and the experimental units to whom it is applied is called the control group.
BLINDING When we know what treatment was assigned, it’s difficult not to let that knowledge influence our assessment of the response, even when we try to be careful. In order to avoid the bias that might result from knowing what treatment was assigned, we use blinding.
BLINDING (CONT.) There are two main classes of individuals who can affect the outcome of the experiment: those who could influence the results (subjects, treatment administrators, technicians) those who evaluate the results (judges, treating physicians, etc.) When all individuals in either one of these classes are blinded, an experiment is said to be single-blind. When everyone in both classes is blinded, the experiment is called double-blind.
PLACEBOS Often simply applying any treatment can induce an improvement. To separate out the effects of the treatment of interest, we can use a control treatment that mimics the treatment itself. A “fake” treatment that looks just like the treatment being tested is called a placebo. Placebos are the best way to blind subjects from knowing whether they are receiving the treatment or not.
PLACEBOS (CONT.) The placebo effect occurs when taking the mock treatment results in a change in the response variable. This highlights both the importance of effective blinding and the importance of comparing treatments with a control. Placebo controls are so effective that you should use them as an essential tool for blinding whenever possible.
THE BEST EXPERIMENTS are usually: randomized. comparative. double-blind. placebo-controlled.
BLOCKING When groups of experimental units are similar, it’s often a good idea to gather them together into blocks. Blocking isolates the variability due to the differences between the blocks so that we can see the differences due to the treatments more clearly. When randomization occurs only within the blocks, we call the design a randomized block design.
BLOCKING (CONT.) In a retrospective or prospective study, subjects are sometimes paired because they are similar in ways not under study. Matching subjects in this way can reduce variability in much the same way as blocking. Example: A retrospective study of music education and grades might match each student who studies an instrument with someone of the same sex who is similar in family income but didn’t study an instrument.
BLOCKING (CONT.) Blocking is the same idea for experiments as stratifying is for sampling. Both methods group together subjects that are similar and randomize within those groups as a way to remove unwanted variation. We use blocks to reduce variability so we can see the effects of the factors; we’re not usually interested in studying the effects of the blocks themselves.
EXAMPLE Recall the experiment about gastric freezing. 1. Now, what does it mean that the experiment was double-blind? 2. Why would you recommend a placebo control? 3. Suppose that researchers suspected that the effectiveness of the gastric freezing treatment might depend on whether a patient had recently developed the peptic ulcer or had been suffering from the condition for a long time. How might the researchers have designed the experiment?
Day 2 ADDING MORE FACTORS It is often important to include multiple factors in the same experiment in order to examine what happens when the factor levels are applied in different combinations.
ADDING MORE FACTORS (CONT.) For example, the following diagram shows a study of the effects of two different factors: fertilizer (three levels) and water (no water or watering daily) on the juiciness and tastiness of tomatoes:
CONFOUNDING When the levels of one factor are associated with the levels of another factor, we say that these two factors are confounded. When we have confounded factors, we cannot separate out the effects of one factor from the effects of the other factor.
LURKING OR CONFOUNDING A lurking variable creates an association between two other variables that tempts us to think that one may cause the other. This can happen in a regression analysis or an observational study. A lurking variable is usually thought of as a prior cause of both y and x that makes it appear that x may be causing y.
LURKING OR CONFOUNDING (CONT.) Confounding can arise in experiments when some other variables associated with a factor has an effect on the response variable. Since the experimenter assigns treatments (at random) to subjects rather than just observing them, a confounding variable can’t be thought of as causing that assignment. A confounding variable, then, is associated in a noncausal way with a factor and affects the response. Because of the confounding, we find that we can’t tell whether any effect we see was caused by our factor or by the confounding factor (or by both working together).
LURKING OR CONFOUNDING (CONT.) Hint: Lurking variables are most common in observational studies. Confounding variables in experiments. In designed experiments, we avoid lurking variables by randomizing subjects and controlling treatments, hence an external variable couldn't affect our treatment variable. Confounding, however, can show up in an experiment either through poor design or just because there is no reasonable way to avoid it.
WHAT CAN GO WRONG? Don’t give up just because you can’t run an experiment. If we can’t perform an experiment, often an observational study is a good choice. Beware of confounding. Use randomization whenever possible to ensure that the factors not in your experiment are not confounded with your treatment levels. Be alert to confounding that cannot be avoided, and report it along with your results.
WHAT CAN GO WRONG? (CONT.) Bad things can happen even to good experiments. Protect yourself by recording additional information! Don’t spend your entire budget on the first run. Try a small pilot experiment before running the full-scale experiment. You may learn some things that will help you make the full-scale experiment better.
RECAP We can recognize sample surveys, observational studies, and randomized comparative experiments. These methods collect data in different ways and lead us to different conclusions. We can identify retrospective and prospective observational studies and understand the advantages and disadvantages of each. Only well-designed experiments can allow us to reach cause-andeffect conclusions. We manipulate levels of treatments to see if the factor we have identified produces changes in our response variable.
RECAP (CONT.) We know the principles of experimental design: Identify and control as many other sources of variability as possible so we can be sure that the variation in the response variable can be attributed to our factor. Try to equalize the many possible sources of variability that cannot be identified by randomly assigning experimental units to treatments. Replicate the experiment on as many subjects as possible. Control the sources of variability we can, and consider blocking to reduce variability from sources we recognize but cannot control.
RECAP (CONT.) We’ve learned the value of having a control group and of using blinding and placebo controls. We can recognize problems posed by confounding variables in experiments and lurking variables in observational studies.
CHAPTER 13 ASSIGNMENTS: PP. 312 – 316 Day 1: # 1, 3, 4, 6, 7, 16 Day 2: # 21, 22, 38, 43 Day 3: # 2, 5, 8, 10, 11, 23, 27, 28, 33, 40, 46 Day 4: # 12, 17, 18, 19, 29, 30 Day 5: # 31, 36, 39, 42